Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Factors influencing protective behavior in the post-COVID-19 period in China: a cross-sectional study

Abstract

Background

The study aimed to explore the factors influencing protective behavior and its association with factors during the post-COVID-19 period in China based on the risk perception emotion model and the protective action decision model (PADM).

Methods

A total of 2830 valid questionnaires were collected as data for empirical analysis via network sampling in China. Structural equation modeling (SEM) was performed to explore the relationships between the latent variables.

Results

SEM indicated that social emotion significantly positively affected protective behavior and intention. Protective behavioral intention had significant direct effects on protective behavior, and the direct effects were also the largest. Government trust did not have a significant effect on protective behavior but did have a significant indirect effect. Moreover, it was found that government trust had the greatest direct effect on social emotion. In addition, we found that excessive risk perception level may directly reduce people’s intention and frequency of engaging in protective behavior, which was not conducive to positive, protective behavior.

Conclusion

In the post-COVID-19 period, theoretical framework constructed in this study can be used to evaluate people’s protective behavior. The government should strengthen its information-sharing and interaction with the public, enhance people’s trust in the government, create a positive social mood, appropriately regulate people's risk perception, and, finally, maintain a positive attitude and intent of protection.

Introduction

Coronavirus disease 19 (COVID-19) has become a major public health emergency of international concern since its first outbreak in late December 2019. The virus outbreak was declared a global pandemic by the World Health Organization (WHO) on March 11, 2020, and the situation remains very serious [1]. The prevention and control of the pandemic urgently require the active participation of governments, health authorities, researchers, and the general public [2]. Most COVID-19 patients have mild symptoms, but some develop serious complications (acute respiratory distress syndrome, myocardial damage, renal insufficiency, etc.), and even after treatment, there can be unimaginable sequelae (heart failure, chronic kidney disease, psychological problems, etc.) [3, 4]. As of June 29, 2021, there were 181.17 million confirmed cases of COVID-19 worldwide, including more than 3.93 million deaths, and the number of new confirmed cases daily was 309048 [5]. Studies have shown that COVID-19 is highly infectious and lethal, significantly more so than SARS-CoV (8098 infected cases and 774 deaths) and MERS-CoV (2494 infected cases and 858 deaths) [6]. As a new infectious disease, our understanding of its natural host and intermediate host is still unclear. The incubation period of COVID-19 can be as long as 24 days and infectious to a certain extent. Moreover, the routes of transmission are diversified. The abovementioned factors pose enormous challenges to the contention of the spread of COVID-19 [3, 7, 8].

Facing the global COVID-19 pandemic, different protective measures have been taken to prevent the further outbreak and spread of the pandemic. Community isolation and closed management have been performed by governments in several countries, such as Bangladesh [9], Serbia [10], Italy [11], South Africa [12], and the Philippines [13]. Furthermore, governments and experts in the prevention and control of infectious diseases have carried out health education and behavioral guidance through various channels [13]. Currently, there is no specific treatment for COVID-19. The process of vaccinating the public against COVID-19 is being sped up in China, but the challenge to achieve herd immunity is tremendous, and the issue of immune persistence has not yet been determined. Importantly, experts recommend maintaining personal protection even for those who have been vaccinated. Therefore, individual protective behavior remains the most immediate and most powerful weapon for reducing COVID-19 infection rates.

In the context of the COVID-19 globalization crisis, China has entered the stage of normal prevention and control since April 29, 2020, which is considered the post-pandemic period [14]. In this period, China’s effective strategy for controlling the outbreak and spread of the pandemic is to prevent “import from outside and rebound from inside.” Anti-infection and anti-rebound have become the top priorities, and this situation is likely to last for a long time. Therefore, in addition to government guidance and efforts, it is important that people continue to adopt protective behavior in the post-pandemic period. According to numerous previous studies, effective measures to reduce COVID-19 infection include isolation at home, keeping a social distance, wearing masks outside, observing personal hygiene, and avoiding crowds [10, 15]. With regard to behavioral decision-making, the public can directly decide whether to adopt protective behavior or not. Previous researchers have also provided a large number of studies on the various factors affecting protective behavior, but the current situation in China reflects a lack of targeted guidance and suggestions. Therefore, it is necessary to understand how public protection behavior in China has emerged in the post-pandemic period. Accordingly, the potential predictors of people’s protective behaviors should be further clarified, so that targeted interventions can be proposed to enhance people’s adherence to the recommended protective behavior.

In the post-COVID-19 pandemic period, this risk-prevention behavior needs to be a part of people’s daily lives and work habits to reduce their own risk of infection. Therefore, based on the risk perception emotion model and protective action decision model (PADM), this study explored the factors that affect people’s protective behavior in the post-pandemic period and puts forward scientific and effective guidance suggestions, which is of great practical significance to the prevention and control of the epidemic in the post-pandemic period.

Theoretical framework

Figure 1 represents the theoretical framework constructed in this study was based on the risk perception emotional model and the PADM. Despite its success in predicting behavior, the research added the factor of government trust, combined with the current situation, to improve our ability to predict behavior in the post-COVID-19 pandemic period.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Theoretical framework

Previous studies indicated that emotional states could reflect the potential of the individual psychological structure, including anger, fear, anxiety, and emotional decision-making [16]. Social emotion is the externalization of personal emotions and also serves as social signals of all of one’s emotions to others. It can involve subjective norms, social influence, and social rules while also highlighting the importance of certain relationships and helping to maintain and restore those relationships when needed, thus acting as a social regulator [17]. Therefore, social emotion will become the driving force for behavioral decisions in social information communication. According to the risk perception emotion model, social emotion is defined as the individual's subjective evaluation and psychological feelings regarding their current social environment. The model holds that emotion-based perceived susceptibility is an important variable that causes individuals to produce behavioral responses. In the face of specific harm or threat, changing the corresponding affective association can effectively change the behavioral response [18]. Consistent with the Assessment Trend Framework (ATF) study [19], Lanciano et al. [20] found that different emotional states increase different risk perceptions. Besides, in previous studies on infectious diseases, such as SARS, empirical results showed that there was a direct relationship between emotion and behavior [21, 22]. However, some scholars have proposed that different emotional states have different mechanisms of influence on behavior. For example, anxiety often comes from the uncertainty of threats, which drives individuals to seek out more information to reduce their inner anxiety [23], while fear usually stimulates or changes one’s own behavior to solve or even avoid problems. Of course, excessive fear may also cause individual avoidance behavior [24]. In addition, behavioral intention was considered to be the closest determinant of behavior in many major theories [25]. Emotion, as a mediating variable or a regulating variable, can directly affect behavioral intention, which has been proved in many fields [26,27,28]. Finally, Zhong et al. [29] emphasized the importance of encouraging a clear understanding of real risks and developing mitigation strategies while taking into account people’s emotional and mental health. Thus, based on the above discussion, we propose the following hypotheses:

  • H1. Social emotion has a significant direct impact on risk perception.

  • H2. Social emotion significantly and directly affects individuals’ protective behavioral intention.

  • H3. Social emotion has a significant and direct influence on individual protective behavior.

Risk perception is a common factor in the risk perception emotion model and the PADM. It is based on the cognition of surrounding risks and reflects people’s direct judgment or subjective prediction of various risks that may occur, such as a dangerous event [18, 30]. Risk perception is divided into individual and social aspects [31]. The former is used to assess one’s own possible risk of infection; the latter is used to assess the possible risk of infection for other people or society as a whole. The decision to engage in protective behavior (protective behavioral intention) is the subjective evaluation of whether an individual is willing to carry out adaptive behavior [32]. Based on the PADM, risk perception indirectly affects individuals’ protective behavior through their behavioral intention. This relationship has been confirmed in many studies on health-related behaviors, and it is believed that risk perception and behavioral intention are important factors for predicting individual adaptive behavior [13, 25, 33, 34]. Furthermore, the PADM in the subsequent expansion reveals that individual risk perception is not a complete intermediary but rather a partial intermediary, which provides evidence for the direct relationship between risk perception and behavior [35]. Existing research shows that the relationship between risk perception and behavior is still unclear. Most scholars believed that there is a positive correlation between the two, meaning that when people were aware of their probability or susceptibility of getting infected with a disease, they stimulated their own behavioral responses and were more likely to comply with various relevant recommended behaviors [36,37,38]. However, Hay et al. [39] believed that there was a two-way relationship between risk perception and behavior, meaning people who perceived a higher risk for themselves would influence other individuals, who engaged in risky behaviors, to make behavioral changes, and risk perception would be reduced once protective behaviors were adopted. At the same time, Aerts et al. [40] proposed that it was crucial to promote the generation of adaptive behaviors by improving people’s risk perception so that the reduction of risk perception would not translate into lower adaptive behaviors. In addition, Wachinger et al. [41] proposed the “risk perception paradox,” arguing that higher risk perception did not necessarily lead to behavioral preparation, nor did it necessarily reduce people’s risky behaviors. Then, what is the relationship between risk perception and behavior in the post-pandemic period? We propose the following hypotheses:

  • H4. Risk perception has a significant direct impact on protective behavioral intention.

  • H5. Risk perception directly affects the individual's protective behavior.

  • H6. Protective behavioral intention directly affects the individual's protective behavior.

In the case of public health emergencies, the government bears the main responsibility for management, prevention, and control, and it is a force that cannot be ignored in emergency management. Early warning, timely monitoring, and rapid prevention and control of the pandemic cannot be achieved without correct government management and guidance. In turn, complying with recommended behavior is dependent upon the government’s epidemic guidelines. In this study, government trust is defined as individuals’ recognition and trust assessment of the government’s crisis management, including information-sharing and pandemic prevention and control [42]. The former mainly means the timely publication of accurate pandemic-related information and continuous publicity of protection information. The latter mainly refers to stopping the spread of the pandemic and providing disease relief. Research in other areas found that trusting policies can also be achieved through people’s emotional responses. The authors believe that irrational actions based on emotional judgments are more likely to be due to people’s lack of knowledge or experience about new, unknown, and ambiguous events [43]. In the case of COVID-19, many researchers have found that government trust affects people’s risk perceptions, protective behaviors, and behavioral intentions [44, 45]. When government trust is high, people believe that various control measures and the information provided by the government are correct and unbiased, which will reduce people’s risk perception level and make them more willing to comply with the protective behaviors recommended by the government. Therefore, we propose the following hypotheses:

  • H7: Government trust significantly affects social emotion.

  • H8: Government trust significantly affects individuals’ risk perceptions.

  • H9: Government trust significantly affects individuals’ protective behavioral intentions.

  • H10: Government trust significantly affects individuals’ protective behaviors.

Methods

Participants

A cross-sectional survey was utilized in this study. Participants were recruited in China from February 1 to February 20, 2021. Before the survey began, we provided an informed consent form, and the questionnaire could only be filled out after the participants had read and agreed to it. Inclusion criteria are as follows: (1) age ≥15 years old, (2) basic reading and writing skills and the ability to use electronic devices, and (3) agreed to participate in this survey. Exclusion criteria are as follows: (1) currently or previously infected with COVID-19 and (2) questionnaire completion times of <0.5 min or > 30 min. Finally, a total of 3437 participants participated in this study.

Data collection

All participants could complete an anonymous online questionnaire (Questionstar: a professional online survey tool). In order to ensure the quality of the questionnaire, we implemented a strict screening mechanism. In the online questionnaire, each question was set as a required item, each participant could only submit it once according to their IP and WeChat ID, and the filling time was monitored in real-time to exclude invalid questionnaires. In addition, the researchers carried out a final quality audit, and participants who passed the audit were given a reward of 5 RMB. Of the 3437 participants, 2830 valid questionnaires were screened systematically and manually, with an availability rate of 82.34%.

Questionnaire

Following the theoretical framework and combining experts’ advice and preliminary survey results, the relevant items were revised and optimized. A formal questionnaire was finally developed and determined to assess the relationship between the public’s risk perception and protective behavior and the potential factors influencing its behavior in the post-pandemic period. The questionnaire consists of six parts: (1) sociodemographic information, including sex, age, residence, province of residence, ethnicity, educational background, marital status, employment status, self-rated economic status, chronic illness, and self-rated health status; (2) social emotion (SE), based on Hareli and Parkinsion [17]; (3) risk perception (RP), based on the WHO [46] and Tyler et al. [31]; (4) protective behavioral intention (PBI), derived from the study of Ajzen et al. [47]; (5) protective behavior (PB), based on a revision of personal hygiene and protection recommendations of the WHO [46] and the State Council of the People’s Republic of China [48]; and (6) government trust, based on studies by Azadi et al. [49] and Samadipour et al. [50]. Each construct contained in the SEM was evaluated using a 5-point Likert scale. Except for protective behavior, for which the scale ranged from 1 = not at all to 5 = very much so, constructs were measured on a scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree. Upon completion of the above information, participants terminated the survey. Each construct and its corresponding items in the study are presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Constructs and corresponding items in the research

According to relevant theories and previous studies, the protective behavior of COVID-19 may be affected by gender, age, ethnicity, and self-rated economic status [51,52,53]. Therefore, this study contained 4 control variables that might affect the protective behavior of COVID-19; these included gender, age, ethnicity, and self-rated economic status. We set a dummy variable for gender, with 1 for male and 2 for female. We used a discrete variable for the ages, with age = 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, and 6 for age under 20, between 21 and 30, between 31 and 40, between 41 and 50, between 51 and 60, and over 61 years, respectively. We used a dummy variable for ethnicity, with 1 for Han nationality and 2 for ethnic minorities. Finally, we used a discrete variable to assess self-rated economic status of our participants with self-rated economic status = 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 for very bad, bad, general, good, and very good, respectively.

Statistical analysis

The statistical analysis was performed using IBM SPSS 25 and AMOS 24. The mean, standard deviation, and extreme value were used for the statistical description of measurement data, while frequencies and percentages were used for the statistical description of enumeration data. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used to evaluate the relationship between the measured and the latent variables. The maximum likelihood estimation method was used to conduct SEM modeling in AMOS 24 software to evaluate the theoretical model of this study. The overall model fitting evaluation indexes and criteria were as follows: chi-square degree of freedom ratio (χ2/DF ratio) < 5.0 [54], root mean square error of approximate (RMSEA) < 0.05, incremental fit index (IFI) > 0.90, goodness of fit index (GFI) > 0.90, adjusted goodness of fit index (AGFI) > 0.90, Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) > 0.90, and comparative fit index (CFI) > 0.90 [55,56,57].

Results

General characteristics of participants

Two thousand eight hundred thirty participants aged 15–83 years (mean=36.64, standard deviation=14.06) were included in the study. The sociodemographic descriptive statistics of the participants are shown in Table 2, indicating that 61.20% of participants were women. Most of the participants were between 21 and 30 years old (33.82%), had an urban residence (61.59%), lived in Chongqing municipality (49.36%), were of Han nationality (96.86%), had obtained a bachelor’s degree or above (40.60%), were married (59.65%), were employed (56.64%), had an average economic status (62.47%), had a good health status (44.98%), and no chronic illnesses (92.05%).

Table 2 Sociodemographic descriptive statistics of participants (N=2830)

Measurement model analysis

We first conducted a CFA to evaluate the validity of the scale. In the measurement model, after the deletion of item PB4, the overall measurement model fitted well. The χ2/DF value was 4.527, which was below the critical value of 5.0. The RMSEA value was 0.035, which was clearly lower than 0.5. The IFI, GFI, AGFI, TLI, and CFI values were 0.990, 0.984, 0.975, 0.986, and 0.990, respectively, which were all greater than the recommended cutoff value of 0.9. These results indicated that the measurement model had good structural validity. The unstandardized factor loadings were significant (P < 0.001). As presented in Table 3, the standardized factor loadings ranged from 0.709 to 0.934, which were clearly all greater than the 0.50 cutoff value. The AVE values ranged from 0.517 to 0.794, also all greater than 0.50. The CR values ranged from 0.762 to 0.920, which were all greater than the cutoff value of 0.70. These results confirmed that the measurement model had good convergence validity [58, 59]. As shown in Table 4, the square roots of AVE were all higher than the correlation coefficients between constructs, indicating that the measurement model had good discriminative validity. Moreover, all constructs in this study were extracted on the basis of previous research theories and modified and supplemented in combination with relevant characteristics of this study and expert opinions, which ensured good content validity of the questionnaire.

Table 3 Factor loadings of influence factors by measurement model test
Table 4 Means, standard deviations, correlations, and square root of AVE (bold font) among model constructs

Finally, Cronbach’s α was utilized to measure the reliability of the measurement model. The Cronbach α values of each construct were all greater than 0.7 (see Table 3), indicating that the internal consistency of the questionnaire was high and that the reliability was also good.

Structural model analysis and hypothesis testing

First, AMOS software was used to conduct the SEM fitting test. The overall adaptability of the model to the sample data was good: χ2/DF= 4.964, RMSEA= 0.037, IFI= 0.979, GFI= 0.977, AGFI= 0.967, TLI= 0.974, and CFI= 0.979. In the structural model, the R2 of PB was 0.525, which was higher than the critical value of medium explanatory power (0.33) [60], showing that all of the variables in the model could explain 52.5% of the PB variation. Therefore, the structural model in this study had a strong explanatory ability and the established model was good.

Next, Fig. 2 shows an estimation of the model for evaluating people's protective behavior during the post-COVID-19 period in China. The results showed that SE was positively associated with PBI (β=0.460, T=17.474, P < 0.001) and PB (β=0.190, T=6.527, P < 0.001). RP had significant direct effects on PBI (β=−0.119, T=−6.429, P < 0.001) and PB (β=−0.123, T=−6.317, P < 0.001). PBI also positively affected PB (β=0.567, T=22.023, P < 0.001). However, SE had no effect on RP (β=−0.027, ns). Thus, hypotheses 2, 3, 4, 5, and 6 were all supported, while hypothesis 1 was not supported. The results showed that the effects of GT on SE (β=0.590, T=27.577, P < 0.001), RP (β=−0.134, T=−4.722, P < 0.001), and PBI (β=0.153, T=6.481, P < 0.001) were significant. However, the effect of GT on PB was not significant (β=−0.021, ns). Thus, hypotheses 7, 8, and 9 were supported, while hypothesis 10 was not supported. In summary, the empirical results of our study supported eight hypotheses and did not support two hypotheses (H1 and H10).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Estimation of the model of protective behavior. Note: *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001.

Finally, a bootstrapping test was utilized to estimate the unstandardized coefficients and significance levels of indirect effects. In total, 5000 bootstrap samples were tested, and the significance level was determined by the percentile method (95% percentile confidence level). The results regarding the direct, indirect, and total effects of the structural model are shown in Table 5. In conclusion, the total effects of SE, RP, PBI, and GT on PB were 0.415, −0.138, 0.558, and 0.304, respectively.

Table 5 Direct effects, indirect effects, and total effects of structural model

Discussion

First of all, the risk perception emotion model and the PADM were used for reference. At the same time, in order to improve the prediction effect of behavior, we added the factor of government trust to construct the theoretical framework of this study. Next, a total of 2,830 questionnaires were collected via an online survey. Finally, SEM was used to analyze and conduct hypothesis testing of the relationships among social emotion, risk perception, protective behavioral intention, protective behavior, and government trust so as to evaluate the factors influencing people’s protective behavior in the post-COVID-19 pandemic period.

The SEM results showed that social emotion had a direct positive effect on people’s protective behavior and willingness to engage in it. It can be concluded that the overall social emotion of people in the post-pandemic period was good, which had a positive impact on their protective decision-making and behavior and also meant that better social emotion helped people form and maintain a positive attitude toward protective measures. Individuals’ emotions and psychological cognition will be affected by external information and the people around them, which can stimulate their understanding and perception of the current pandemic to a certain extent [61]. Interestingly, social emotion had no direct effect on risk perception. In other words, China’s pandemic prevention and control have entered the post-pandemic stage, and positive social emotion has not significantly stimulated people's risk perception. One possible reason for this observation was that people believed that the current social environment was good and that the infection risk was relatively low. However, considering that the epidemic had not completely disappeared and that it may occur again, people had not completely eliminated their worries, anxiety, or panic about the pandemic and still maintained a certain level of risk perception [62]. Therefore, better social emotion had no significant impact on people’s risk perception.

Risk perception was negatively correlated with protective behavior and willingness to engage in it, which is consistent with Guo et al.’s findings [34]. This meant that excessive risk perception was not conducive to the adoption of recommended adaptive behaviors and may produce non-recommended behaviors or negative behaviors [63]. It can also be argued that those with high level of trust in the government were most likely to believe that their living environment was relatively safe despite a rebound of the pandemic, that their risk of contracting the disease was low or that they were less susceptible to the pandemic, and that their level of risk perception was weaker than they were during the COVID-19 pandemic period [6]. Therefore, it is necessary for relevant departments to guide individuals to maintain a moderate level of risk perception and trust the government to adopt active protective behavior.

The behavioral intention variable is considered to be the best predictor of individuals’ compliance with the recommended behavior [64]. The strength of behavioral willingness is closely related to whether people are willing to cooperate and how closely they cooperate. In other words, long-term, effective compliance with the recommended protective behavior depends on people's willingness to engage in such behavior. The SEM showed that protective behavioral intention could directly and positively influences people’s protective behavior, which is consistent with many prior research results [13, 65]. The results suggested that behavioral intention was the most direct and most important factor influencing people’s protective behavior in the post-COVID-19 pandemic period. Hence, in order to better guide people to follow the official recommended protective behaviors, the government should give full consideration to people’s willingness to engage in these and enhance people’s cooperation, which can promote the effectiveness and sustainability of the implementation of these behaviors to a certain extent and can also contribute to the formation of better hygiene habits in daily life.

The study found that the direct effect of government trust on social emotion, risk perception, and willingness to engage in protective behavior was significant. Government trust was not directly related to protective behavior, but it had a large indirect effect. It can be said that because of trust in the government, various prevention and control measures, such as COVID-19 vaccination, carried out by the government, will stimulate people’s social emotion, arouse their concern and attention regarding these events, accelerate their correct understanding and perception thereof, and reduce their fear and anxiety. Finally, it can lead people to a positive response to the pandemic [45].

We even observed that the total effects of social emotion, risk perception, willingness to engage in protective behavior, and government trust regarding protective behavior were 0.415, -0.138, 0.558, and 0.304, respectively. From what has been discussed above, our suggestion is to establish good, cooperative relations between the government and the public. On the one hand, the government should provide early warning, monitoring, and epidemic prevention and control. On the other hand, the government should do a good job with their information-sharing and interactions with the public to strengthen people’s trust in the government and create a good social atmosphere. This may lead people to maintain the correct attitude of prevention and control, reasonably responding to all kinds of emergencies.

Although this study has provided findings and recommendations, there are still some potential limitations and future directions. First, due to the influence of the pandemic, the recruitment of the survey participants was challenging, and only online recruitment could be conducted through the method of non-probability sampling. However, we collected a large number of samples to reduce deviation as much as possible. Second, we only collected data from the late period of the COVID-19 pandemic in China, and this study mainly examines the protective behavior of Chinese residents aged 15 years and above. Therefore, the application and extension of the theoretical model of this study to evaluate protective behavior regarding COVID-19 toward other countries and regions needs to be further explored. Third, due to the time lag, we adopted a cross-sectional survey design so that the dynamic changes of the influence of government trust, social sentiment, and protective behavioral intention on behavior in different periods were not tracked longitudinally.

Conclusion

In this study, a theoretical framework was constructed based on the risk perception emotion model and the PADM. Survey data of 2830 participants were collected. This empirical study explored the factors and processes influencing people’s protective behavior in the post-COVID-19 pandemic period via SEM. The results revealed that social emotion had a significant positive effect on protective behavior and willingness to engage in them. Protective behavior intention had a direct influence on protective behavior, and this influence was the largest. Government trust had no significant positive effect on protective behavior, but it had a significant indirect effect. Moreover, we found that government trust had the greatest direct effect on social emotion. Furthermore, we observed that excessive risk perception levels might reduce people’s willingness and frequency of engaging in protective behavior, which was not conducive to positive protective behavior. Thus, based on these findings, we suggest that the government should strengthen their information-sharing and interaction with the public, enhance people’s trust in the government, create a good social emotion, properly regulate people’s risk perception, and finally, maintain a positive attitude and intent of protection.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. World Health Organization. Geneva, Switzerland Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) situation reports 2021 [updated 2021.06.30. Available from: https://www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019/situation-reports.

    Google Scholar 

  2. World Health Organization. Geneva, Switzerland Coronavirus disease (COVID-19) advice for the public 2021 [Available from: https://www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019/advice-for-public.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Wang Y, Wang Y, Chen Y, Qin Q. Unique epidemiological and clinical features of the emerging 2019 novel coronavirus pneumonia (COVID-19) implicate special control measures. J Med Virol. 2020;92(6):568–76. https://doi.org/10.1002/jmv.25748.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Allegrante JP, Auld ME, Natarajan S. Preventing COVID-19 and its sequela: “There Is No Magic Bullet... It's Just Behaviors”. Am J Prev Med. 2020;59(2):288–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2020.05.004.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. World Health Organization. WHO Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19) Global Situation 2021 [updated 2021.06.30. Available from: https://covid19.who.int/].

    Google Scholar 

  6. Ahmad M, Iram K, Jabeen G. Perception-based influence factors of intention to adopt COVID-19 epidemic prevention in China. Environ Res. 2020;190:109995. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2020.109995.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Riou J, Althaus CL. Pattern of early human-to-human transmission of Wuhan 2019 novel coronavirus (2019-nCoV), December 2019 to January 2020. Euro Surveill. 2020;25(4):7-11. https://doi.org/10.2807/1560-7917.ES.2020.25.4.2000058.

  8. World Health Organization. Q&A on coronaviruses (COVID-19) 2021 [Available from: https://www.who.int/news-room/q-a-detail/q-a-coronaviruses].

    Google Scholar 

  9. Abir T, Kalimullah NA, Osuagwu UL, Yazdani DMN, Mamun AA, Husain T, et al. Factors associated with the perception of risk and knowledge of contracting the SARS-Cov-2 among adults in Bangladesh: analysis of online surveys. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(14):5252. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17145252.

  10. Cvetković VM, Nikolić N, Radovanović Nenadić U, Öcal A, K Noji E, Zečević M. Preparedness and preventive behaviors for a pandemic disaster caused by COVID-19 in Serbia. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(11):4124. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17114124.

  11. Gallè F, Sabella EA, Da Molin G, De Giglio O, Caggiano G, Di Onofrio V, et al. Understanding knowledge and behaviors related to CoViD-19 epidemic in Italian undergraduate students: The EPICO Study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(10):3481. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17103481.

  12. Kim AW, Nyengerai T, Mendenhall E. Evaluating the mental health impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic: perceived risk of COVID-19 infection and childhood trauma predict adult depressive symptoms in urban South Africa. Psychol Med. 2020:1–13. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720003414.

  13. Prasetyo YT, Castillo AM, Salonga LJ, Sia JA, Seneta JA. Factors affecting perceived effectiveness of COVID-19 prevention measures among Filipinos during Enhanced Community Quarantine in Luzon, Philippines: Integrating Protection Motivation Theory and extended Theory of Planned Behavior. Int J Infect Dis. 2020;99:312–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijid.2020.07.074.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. State Council. Fighting COVID-19: China in Action. 2020.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Hutchins HJ, Wolff B, Leeb R, Ko JY, Odom E, Willey J, et al. COVID-19 Mitigation behaviors by age group - United States, April-June 2020. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2020;69(43):1584–90. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.mm6943e4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Chervonsky E, Hunt C. Suppression and expression of emotion in social and interpersonal outcomes: a meta-analysis. Emotion. 2017;17(4):669–83. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. HARELI S, PARKINSON B. What's social about social emotions? J Theory Soc Behav. 2008;38(2):131–56. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5914.2008.00363.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Peters EM, Burraston B, Mertz CK. An emotion-based model of risk perception and stigma susceptibility: cognitive appraisals of emotion, affective reactivity, worldviews, and risk perceptions in the generation of technological stigma. Risk Anal. 2004;24(5):1349–67. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00531.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Lerner JS, Keltner D. Beyond valence: toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. Cognition & Emotion. 2000;14(4):473–93. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999300402763.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Lanciano T, Graziano G, Curci A, Costadura S, Monaco A. Risk perceptions and psychological effects during the Italian COVID-19 emergency. Front Psychol. 2020;11:580053. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.580053.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Lau JT, Yang X, Tsui H, Kim JH. Monitoring community responses to the SARS epidemic in Hong Kong: from day 10 to day 62. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003;57(11):864–70. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech.57.11.864.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Leung GM, Ho LM, Chan SK, Ho SY, Bacon-Shone J, Choy RY, et al. Longitudinal assessment of community psychobehavioral responses during and after the 2003 outbreak of severe acute respiratory syndrome in Hong Kong. Clin Infect Dis. 2005;40(12):1713–20. https://doi.org/10.1086/429923.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. So J. A further extension of the Extended Parallel Process Model (E-EPPM): implications of cognitive appraisal theory of emotion and dispositional coping style. Health Commun. 2013;28(1):72–83. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2012.708633.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Lazarus RS. Progress on a cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion. Am Psychol. 1991;46(8):819–34. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.46.8.819.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Fishbein M, Ajzen I, Belief A. Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: an introduction to theory and research. Contemp Sociol. 1977;6(2):244-5. https://doi.org/10.2307/2065853.

  26. Ding J. Psychological empowerment and turnover intention among university counselors: emotional exhaustion as a mediator and emotion regulation as a moderator. Curr Psychol. 2021:1–7. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02079-7.

  27. Zhang CB, Hou RJ, Mu-Nong YI, Business SO. The relationship of perceived justice, emotion and behavior intention after online shopping service recovery——a moderated mediation model based on relationship quality. J Shanxi Finance Econ Univ. 2014;36(01):54-64.https://doi.org/10.13781/j.cnki.1007-9556.2014.01.005.

  28. Li S, Jiang Y, Cheng B, Scott N. The effect of flight delay on customer loyalty intention: the moderating role of emotion regulation. J Hospitality Tourism Manag. 2021;47(3):72–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhtm.2021.03.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Zhong Y, Liu W, Lee TY, Zhao H, Ji J. Risk perception, knowledge, information sources and emotional states among COVID-19 patients in Wuhan, China. Nurs Outlook. 2021;69(1):13–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.outlook.2020.08.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Lindell MK, Perry RW. Communicating environmental risk in multiethnic communities Thousand Oaks: communicating environmental risk in multiethnic communities; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Tyler TR. Impact of directly and indirectly experienced events: the origin of crime-related judgments and behaviors. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;39(1):13–28. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.39.1.13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Glasman LR, Albarracín D. Forming attitudes that predict future behavior: a meta-analysis of the attitude-behavior relation. Psychol Bull. 2006;132(5):778–822. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.778.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Wang FP, Wei JP, Shi X. Compliance with recommended protective actions during an H7N9 emergency: a risk perception perspective. Disasters. 2018;42(2):207–32. https://doi.org/10.1111/disa.12240.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Guo Q, Yao N, Zhu W. How consumers’ perception and information processing affect their acceptance of genetically modified foods in China: a risk communication perspective. Food Res Int. 2020;137:109518. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodres.2020.109518.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Lindell MK, Hwang SN. Households’ perceived personal risk and responses in a multihazard environment. Risk Anal. 2008;28(2):539–56. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2008.01032.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Fakih MG, Sturm LK, Fakih RR. Overcoming COVID-19: addressing the perception of risk and transitioning protective behaviors to habits. Infect Control Hosp Epidemiol. 2021;42(4):489–90. https://doi.org/10.1017/ice.2020.284.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Li S, Feng B, Liao W, Pan W. Internet use, risk awareness, and demographic characteristics associated with engagement in preventive behaviors and testing: cross-sectional survey on COVID-19 in the United States. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(6):e19782. https://doi.org/10.2196/19782.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Huang J, Cao W, Wang H, Wang Z. Affect path to flood protective coping behaviors using SEM based on a survey in Shenzhen, China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(3):940. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17030940.

  39. Hay JL, Ostroff J, Burkhalter J, Li Y, Quiles Z, Moadel A. Changes in cancer-related risk perception and smoking across time in newly-diagnosed cancer patients. J Behav Med. 2007;30(2):131–42. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-007-9094-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Aerts C, Revilla M, Duval L, Paaijmans K, Chandrabose J, Cox H, et al. Understanding the role of disease knowledge and risk perception in shaping preventive behavior for selected vector-borne diseases in Guyana. PLoS Negl Trop Dis. 2020;14(4):e0008149. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pntd.0008149.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Wachinger G, Renn O, Begg C, Kuhlicke C. The risk perception paradox--implications for governance and communication of natural hazards. Risk Anal. 2013;33(6):1049–65. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01942.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Duan T, Jiang H, Deng X, Zhang Q, Wang F. Government Intervention, risk perception, and the adoption of protective action recommendations: evidence from the COVID-19 prevention and control experience of China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(10):3387. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17103387.

  43. Rodriguez-Sanchez C, Schuitema G, Claudy M, Sancho-Esper F. How trust and emotions influence policy acceptance: the case of the Irish water charges. Br J Soc Psychol. 2018;57(3):610–29. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12242.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Gesser-Edelsburg A, Cohen R, Hijazi R, Abed Elhadi Shahbari N. Analysis of public perception of the Israeli government's early emergency instructions regarding COVID-19: online survey Study. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(5):e19370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Siegrist M, Luchsinger L, Bearth A. The impact of trust and risk perception on the acceptance of measures to reduce COVID-19 cases. Risk Anal. 2021;41(5):787–800. https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.13675.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. WHO Regional Office For Europe. COVID-19 Snapshot MOnitoring (COSMO Standard): monitoring knowledge, risk perceptions, preventive behaviours, and public trust in the current coronavirus outbreak—WHO standard protocol; 2020 [updated 2020.08.16]. Available from:. https://doi.org/10.23668/PSYCHARCHIVES.2782.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  47. Ajzen I. Constructing a TPB questionnaire: conceptual and methodological considerations; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  48. State Council of the People’s Republic of China. COVID-19 prevention and control program. 7th ed; 2021. [updated 2020.10.16]. Available from: http://www.gov.cn/xinwen/2020-09/15/content_5543680.htm

    Google Scholar 

  49. Azadi Y, Yazdanpanah M, Mahmoudi H. Understanding smallholder farmers’ adaptation behaviors through climate change beliefs, risk perception, trust, and psychological distance: evidence from wheat growers in Iran. J Environ Manage. 2019;250:109456. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvman.2019.109456.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Samadipour E, Ghardashi F, Aghaei N. Evaluation of risk perception of COVID-19 disease: a community-based participatory study. Disaster Med Public Health Prep. 2020:1–8. https://doi.org/10.1017/dmp.2020.311.

  51. Dini BM, Bodroa B. COVID-19 protective behaviors are forms of prosocial and unselfish behaviors. Front Psychol. 2021;12:647710. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.647710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Papageorge NW, Zahn MV, Belot M, Broek-Altenburg E, Choi S, Jamison JC, et al. Socio-demographic factors associated with self-protecting behavior during the Covid-19 pandemic. NBER Working Papers. 2020.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  53. Hearne BN, Nio M. Understanding how race, ethnicity, and gender shape mask-wearing adherence during the COVID-19 pandemic: evidence from the COVID impact survey. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2021;1-8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40615-020-00941-1. Online ahead of print.

  54. Wheaton B, Muthén B, Alwin DF, Summers GF. Assessing reliability and stability in panel models. Sociol Methodol. 1977;8:84–136. https://doi.org/10.2307/270754.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Chin J, Jiang B, Mufidah I, Persada S, Noer B. The investigation of consumers’ behavior intention in using green skincare products: a pro- environmental behavior model approach. Sustainability. 2018;10(11):3922. https://doi.org/10.3390/su10113922.

  56. Hair JF, Black WC, Babin BJ, Anderson RE. Multivariate data analysis: a global perspective (7th ed). Upper Saddle River: Pearson; 2010. ISBN: 0135153093.

  57. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equation Model. 1999;6(1):1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Hair JF. Multivariate data analysis; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Fornell C, Larcker DF. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J Mark Res. 1981;18(1):39–50. https://doi.org/10.1177/002224378101800104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Chin WW. The partial least squares approach to structural equation modeling. In: Marcouliders GA (Ed) Mod Methods Bus Res. 1998;295(2):295-336.

  61. Sjberg L. Emotions and risk perception. Risk Manage. 2007;9(4):223–37. https://doi.org/10.1057/palgrave.rm.8250038.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Si H, Shen L, Liu W, Wu G. Uncovering people's mask-saving intentions and behaviors in the post-COVID-19 period: evidence from China. Sustain Cities Soc. 2021;65:102626. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scs.2020.102626.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Ben-Ezra M, Sun S, Hou WK, Goodwin R. The association of being in quarantine and related COVID-19 recommended and non-recommended behaviors with psychological distress in Chinese population. J Affect Disord. 2020;275:66–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.06.026.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Bentler PM, Speckart G. Models of attitude–behavior relations. Psychological Review. 1979;86(5):452–64. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.86.5.452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Tran VT, Ravaud P. COVID-19-related perceptions, context and attitudes of adults with chronic conditions: results from a cross-sectional survey nested in the ComPaRe e-cohort. PLoS One. 2020;15(8):e0237296. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237296.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank all individuals who volunteered to participate and investigators for their help in the data collection during this research and are grateful to International Science Editing services in manuscript language polishing.

Funding

This work was supported by the Chongqing Medical University New Coronavirus Emergency Research Special Project (grant numbers: CQMUNCP0305). The sponsor was not involved in study design; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

GQS, as the first author, was responsible for the paper’s conceptualization, designing the framework, and running SEM in AMOS and prepared the manuscript. WH, HL, XYL, and MZM conducted a survey design, collected the data, and interpreted the data. XNZ, as the corresponding author, was responsible for revising it critically for important intellectual content and finalizing the manuscript. All authors significantly contributed to the revisions of the manuscript. The authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Xiaoni Zhong.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Chongqing Medical University. The questionnaire was filled in anonymously. Informed consent was obtained by participants clicking the option on the question of “I confirm that I voluntarily participate in this survey.” All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Shi, G., Zhong, X., He, W. et al. Factors influencing protective behavior in the post-COVID-19 period in China: a cross-sectional study. Environ Health Prev Med 26, 95 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12199-021-01015-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12199-021-01015-2

Keywords