Skip to main content

Simple reaction time and obesity in children: whether there is a relationship?

Abstract

Objective

Reaction time (RT) testing is one of the oldest diagnostic methods used in modern psychology, and is known as simple and sensitive cognitive test. It has been recently reported that RT is related to obesity in young, adult and elderly individuals. However, most of the studies included small sample of participants, used just body mass index (BMI) as body obesity index, and did not consider some potential confounders such as age, socioeconomic status and physical activity in their studies. Furthermore, there is little and contradictory results for children. Therefore, the present study aimed to examine the relationship between RT and weight status in a sample of children.

Methods

Three hundred and fifty four 9–12 year old schoolboys underwent standard anthropometry, and various simple RT tests.

Results

After controlling for potential confounders no significant relationship was observed between audio-RT (RTA) and clinical RT (RTclin) with BMI, %fat, waist circumference (WC) and waist to height ratio (WHtR) (P > 0.05). But, significant relationship (β = 0.18; P = 0.02) was observed between visual-RT (RTV) and %fat (but not BMI, WC and WHtR).

Conclusions

Among the various simple RT tasks and central and total body obesity indices, just significant relationship was observed between %fat and RTV in the schoolboys. According to the results, it is concluded that RT impairment due to obesity may less be observed, or may not be observed for some types of RT tasks and obesity indices during childhood.

Introduction

Reaction time (RT) testing is one of the oldest diagnostic methods used in modern psychology, and the first examinations of this parameter dates back to the nineteenth century [1]. RT tests are known as simple and sensitive cognitive test in both healthy individuals and patients [2]. It is the time interval between the using of a stimulus and the appearance of quick voluntary response by an individual. It involves three phases as follows: a) processing of the stimulus, b) making decision about it and c) programming a response. Therefore, RT measurement includes: 1- The sensory neural code latency traversing both in central and peripheral pathways, 2- Both cognitive and Perceptive processing, 3- A motor signal traversing both peripheral and central neuronal structures 4- And eventually the latency in the end effectors activation such as muscle activation [3]. Speed of human’s information processing and its quality can be evaluated by using one (simple RT) or more (Choice RT) stimuli, and from an executive functions perspective both tasks are a measure of sustained attention; however, choice RT task put greater stress on the decision making and leads prolonged RT than simple RT [4].

It has been shown that some factors such as age [5,6,7], caffeine and some drugs [8, 9], illnesses [10], socioeconomic status (SES) [6, 7, 11], and physical activity (PA) lifestyle patterns [6, 7, 12, 13] are associated with RT. There is also some evidence has recently been reported by researches underlying relationship between obesity and RT in young, adult and elderly individuals [14,15,16,17,18,19,20]. The evidence suggests that overweight/obese individuals are inferior while performing RT compared with their healthy weight peers [14,15,16,17,18,19,20]. But, contradictory results underlying the relationship between obesity and RT have been reported for children [5, 6].

Regarding the relationship between obesity and RT that have been reported recently some points should be noted. For instance, most of the previous studies underlying the relationship between RT and obesity included small sample of participants and/or used just body mass index (BMI) as the obesity index [15,16,17,18,19,20], while BMI can be influenced by a number of factors and indices, such as central obesity, which is more closely linked to some adverse health outcomes than BMI [21], and therefore obesity indices may differentially relate to changes in cognitive function over time. In addition some previous similar studies did not consider some potential confounders such as age, socioeconomic status (SES) and PA lifestyle patterns [15,16,17,18,19,20], whereas it has been reported that SES is strongly correlated with cognitive ability and achievement during childhood and beyond [11]; and it has been shown that more physically active individuals are better able to respond quickly to a stimulus presented to [6, 7, 12] and are capable of allocating more attentional resources toward the environment and process information more quickly [13].

In this context the purpose of the present study was to examine whether there is significant relationship between various RT tasks and obesity indices among a relatively large numbers of children after controlling for some potential confounders such as age, SES and PA.

Methods

Participants

During 2013–2014, a cross-sectional data were drawn from a sample of three hundred and fifty four, 9–12 year old schoolboys of three schools, in the center of Ardabil Province, North West of Iran. Three schools were selected randomly from a list of boys’ urban public schools (N = 130). The nature and purpose of the study were explained to all schoolboys (n = 964) before invitation for participating in the present study. Children were excluded of the study if they were identified with known presence of chronic disease or using any medication which could affect RT [9, 10]. Children who were invited and passed the exclusion criteria of the study and gave their consent verbally (n = 605) were given written consent forms for their parents approval. Total of four hundred fifty three signed consent forms were collected and the owners of the forms were recruited into the study. Nonetheless, at the end of the study, complete data were collected from 354 participants [dropped data because of illness, absence and withdrawal for some personal reasons etc.] (Fig. 1). Age of the boys was determined from their date of birth in their school register. The anthropometric variables and RT tests were measured in the empty room. The measurements took place when a participant was at rest. For this mean, during the physical education lessons selected boys were requested to undergo the measurements. All measurements were performed during the winter and spring of the year 2013. General characteristics of the participants are indicated in Table 1. The present study was approved by the Human Ethics Committee of the Ardabil Department of Education, and the experiment was performed in accordance with the ethical standards of the committee and with the Helsinki Declaration.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow diagram demonstrating the study methodology

Table 1 General characteristics of the boys (n = 354)

Anthropometric variables and obesity indices

Height of the participants was measured barefoot in the Frankfurt horizontal plane with a telescopic height measuring instrument (Type SECA 225) to the nearest one mm. Weight of the participants was measured in without shoes and underwear with an electronic scale (Type SECA 861) to the nearest 0.1 kg.

BMI and %fat were measured as the total body obesity indices and waist circumference (WC) and waist to height ratio (WHtR) were measured as the central body obesity indices. BMI was calculated as body weight in kilograms divided by the square of height in meters. WC was measured midway between the superior border of the iliac crest and the lowest rib with an inelastic measuring tape at the end of normal expiration to the nearest 0.1 mm. WC was divided by the height to determine the waist to height ratio (WHtR). Lange skinfold caliper was used to assess triceps and calf skinfold thickness [22] on the right side of the body for all the subjects. The average of three measures was calculated for each site and then %fat was calculated according to the equations [22].

$$ \mathbf{Boys}\ \%\mathbf{fat} = 0.735\ \left( sum\ of\ average\ skinfolds\right) + 1.0 $$

Reaction time (RT) tests

All participants were made familiar with procedure of the RT tests and were requested to avoid using caffeine containing drinks [8] and any acute PA [12]. The boys were made to sit comfortably in a chair and were motivated to better their results as much as possible, using the dominant hand, with a protocol of 5 practice trials for each RT tests followed by data acquisition trials. Data from practice trials were not included to analyses. In our experiments we used rest break times (5 min) between each RT test to prevent fatigue [23].

Audio Reaction Time (RTA) and Visual Reaction time (RTV) tests

RT software which was installed on a laptop was used for obtaining both RTA and RTV which had an inter-stimulus interval from1000 to 3000ms, response range from 150 to 1500 ms, and a display accuracy of 0.001 s. By starting each RT test each subject completed 40 RTV and 40 RTA executions. For performing RTV the subject was requested to press a default key (spacebar) as soon as possible, using the index finger, which was in contact with the key, when a large dot (highlighting green circles against a yellow background) appeared on the monitor. For executing RTA the subject was requested to press the key every time he heard a “beep” sound. Headphone was provided for clarity of sound. After completing each test, average of performances for each test were recorded for each subject.

Clinical Reaction Time (RTclin)

In addition to the above computerized simple RT tests we measured a new clinical measure of RT (RTclin) which has been recently developed and validated [24, 25]. Each boy participated in simple RTclin testing, using the RTclin apparatus, which has been described elsewhere [5,6,7, 24, 25] but is repeated here for convenience of the reader. The apparatus for measuring RTclin is a measuring 0.8 m long stick, marked in 5 mm increments embedded in a weighted rubber disk. Each boy sat with the dominant forearm resting on a horizontal desk surface, such that the proximal edge of the hypothenar eminence was positioned at the edge of the desk. The apparatus was held by the examiner vertically, with the weighted disk put inside the boy’s open hand, such that the superior surface of the weighted disk was aligned with the plane of the boy’s first 2 digits and no part of his hand was in contact with the weighted disk. The apparatus was dropped by the examiner at predetermined, randomly assigned time intervals of between two and five seconds to prevent the boy from anticipating the time of drop. Then, the boy tried to catch the apparatus as quickly as possible after it began to drop. In the event of an anticipatory catch before the apparatus was dropped, the examiner restarted the random-delay interval count before dropping the apparatus. If the device was dropped by a boy that trial did not include in the calculation of mean RTclin. The boys were tested using the dominant hand with a protocol of 10 data acquisition trials. The distance the device fell before being caught by the boy was recorded in meters (m), and used to calculate RTclin in seconds (s) for each trial using the formula for a body falling under the influence of gravity (t = 0.45 × √d); where “d” is distance (m) and “t” is time (s) [24, 25]. Mean baseline RTclin values were calculated for each boy.

Potential covariates/confounders

Physical activity (PA)

PA for the children was measured using the PA Questionnaire - Children (PAQ-C) a reliable and valid measure of PA for children during the school year [26,27,28] with some alternations to suit to our society [29, 30]. Children were requested to fill out the questionnaire under their parents’ supervision.

Socioeconomic status (SES)

SES was computed from parents’ education and occupational status which is explained previously [6, 7, 30, 31].

Statistics

Descriptive statistics were run on all variables. Data were screened for problems of skew, kurtosis, and outliers. Initial Pearson product–moment correlations were conducted on relationship between RT tests and age, SES and PA score. Any variable exhibiting a significant correlation with the dependent variable (RT) was included as a covariate/confounder in the analyses. For finding the relationship between the RT tests and the obesity indices hierarchical regression analysis was conducted in 2 steps. In the first step confounding variables (age, SES and PA) were entered, and in the second step obesity indices were entered, separately. All calculations were performed using SPSS v.21.0 software for Windows (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL, USA). The significance level was set at p < 0.05.

Results

Pearson correlation (Table 2) indicated significant negative relationship between age and RTV, RTA and RTclin (P < 0.01). Significant negative relationship was observed between SES and RTV, RTA and RTclin (P < 0.01). Although, negative relationship was observed between PA and the RT tests, only significant relationship was observed between PA and RTclin (P < 0.05).

Table 2 Pearson correlation between the RT tests and age, SES and PA

Multiple linear regression analysis (Table 3) indicated significant negative relationship between age and the RT tests (P < 0.01). Significant negative relationship was observed between RTclin and SES (P < 0.05). Significant negative relationship was found between PA and RTclin and RTV (P > 0.05). Linear regression analysis after adjustment for the potential confounders (step 2) indicated significant relationship between RTV and %fat (P < 0.05). No significant relationship was observed between RTV, BMI, WHtR and WC (P > 0.05). In addition regression analysis indicated no significant relationship between RTA and RTclin with the all obesity indices (P > 0.05).

Table 3 Relationship between the RT tests with possible confounders (step 1) and the obesity indices (step 2)

Discussion

The results indicated that among the various obesity indices and simple RT tests, and after controlling for the possible confounders only significant relationship was observed between RTV and %fat. No significant relationship was observed between RTV, BMI and central obesity indices; and no significant relationship was observed between RTclin and RTA to the obesity indices.

In the previous studies among schoolboys no significant relationship was observed between RTclin and various obesity indices (BMI, %fat, WC and WHtR), which the present study results replicate the previous study results [5, 6]. It has been suggested that type of the RT task might play an important role in the relationship between weight status and RT [5,6,7]. For instance, some recent studies reported contradictory results based on the relationship between type of task performance and obesity, and discrepancy results have been shown between healthy weight and obese children for different types of RT, and it has been concluded that “childhood obesity is negatively and selectively associated with prefrontal inhibitory control” [14, 32].

Although, several recent studies have reported positive relationship between RT and obesity (measured by BMI) among young, adult and older people [15,16,17,18,19,20], however, we observed that among a relatively larger sample of participants, using various obesity indices and RT tests and controlling for potential confounders just RTV was significantly related to %fat. In this context it should be stated that subcutaneous fat percentage as an overall body obesity index has been reported as a better body obesity predictor than BMI and has been concluded that BMI as a measure of total body obesity does not account for varying proportions of fat, muscle mass, and bone or the distribution of body fat [33, 34]. However, to the best of our knowledge little studies underlying the relationship between obesity and RT have used subcutaneous fat percentage as well as central obesity.

Interestingly, we observed negative (but not significant) relationship between RTclin and obesity in the children. Our results are according to some recent studies that have reported it is not necessarily appropriate to consider body adiposity as a negative factor influencing neuromuscular RT performance, and in contrast body lipid reserves are integral to the development of the nervous system (e.g., the development of myelin nerve heath) and thus better RT, even amongst individuals within the healthy weight ranges [35]. Therefore, according to the results of the present study and literature [5,6,7, 17, 32, 35, 36] it is plausible that RT impairment due to obesity lees be observed during childhood, or may not be observed for some types of RT tasks and obesity indices.

However, since the study has several limitations, results need to be interpreted with caution. For example, the cross-sectional nature of the study limits the possibility to draw conclusions regarding causality of any of the observed associations in the present study. Furthermore, the present study did not include subjects of both sexes. Therefore, for more reliable results about the relationship between RT and weight status, additional studies by using larger sample size, various age groups, RT tests, obesity indices and in both sexes are needed.

Abbreviations

BMI:

Body mass index

PA:

Physical activity

RT:

Reaction time

RTA:

Audio reaction time

RTclin :

Clinical reaction time

RTV:

Visual reaction time

SES:

Socioeconomic status

WC:

Waist circumference

WHtR:

Waist to height circumference

References

  1. Merkel J. Die zeitlichen Verhältnisse der Willensthähigkeit. Philos Stud. 1885;2:73–127.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Jakobsen LH, Sorensen JM, Rask IK, Jensen BS, Kondrup J. Validation of reaction time as a measure of cognitive function and quality of life in healthy subjects and patients. Nutrition. 2011;27:561–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Botwinik J, Thompson LW. Premotor and motor components of reaction time. J Exp Psychol. 1966;71:9–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Magill RA. Motor learning and control: Concepts and applications. 8th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill Companies; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Esmaeilzadeh S. Reaction time: does it relate to weight status in children? HOMO-J Compar Hum Biol. 2014;65:171–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Moradi A, Esmaeilzadeh S. Association between reaction time, speed and agility in schoolboys. Sport Sci Health. 2015;11:251–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Esmaeilzadeh S, Farzizadeh R, Kalantari HA, Mahmoudi A. Does obesity cause reaction time impairment in youth? HOMO-J Compar Hum Biol, Under review.

  8. Durlach PJ, Edmunds R, Howard L, Tipper SP. A rapid effect of caffeinated beverages on two choice reaction time tasks. Nutr Neurosci. 2002;5:433–42.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Spencer SV, Hawk-Jr LW, Richards JB, Shiels K, Pelham Jr WE, Waxmonsky JG. Stimulant treatment reduces lapses in attention among children with ADHD: The effects of methylphenidate on intra-individual response time distributions. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37:805–16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Smith A, Brice C, Leach A, Tiley M, Williamson S. Effects of upper respiratory tract illnesses in a working population. Ergonomics. 2004;47:363–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Noble KG, Norman MF, Farah MJ. Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Dev Sci. 2005;8:74–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Murray NP, Russoniello C. Acute Physical Activity on Cognitive Function: A Heart Rate Variability Examination. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2012;37:219–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Sibley BA, Etnier JL. The relationship between physical activity and cognition in children: A meta-analysis. Pediatr Exer Sci. 2003;15:243–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Kamijo K, Pontifex MB, Khan NA, Raine LB, Scudder MR, Drollette ES, et al. The negative association of childhood obesity to cognitive control of action monitoring. Cere Cortex. 2014;24:654–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Skurvydas A, Gutnik B, Zuoza AK, Nash D, Zuoziene IJ, Mickeviciene D. Relationship between simple reaction time and body mass index. Homo-J compar Hum boil. 2009;60:77–85.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Deore DN, Surwase SP, Masroor S, Khan ST, Kathore V. A cross sectional study on the relationship between the body mass index (BMI) and the audiovisual reaction time (ART). J Clin Diag Res. 2012;6:1466–8.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Gunstad J, Paul RH, Cohaen RA, Tate DF, Gordon E. Obesity is associated with memory deficits in young and middle aged adults. J Eat Weight Disord. 2006;11:15–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Nene AS, Pazare PA, Sharma KD. A study of relation between body mass index and simple reaction time in healthy young females. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. 2011;55:288–91.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Nikam LH, Gadkari JV. Effect of age, gender and body mass index on visual and auditory reaction times in Indian population. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. 2012;56:94–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Gentier I, Augustijn M, Deforche B, Tanghe A, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Lenoir M, D’Hondt E. A comparative study of performance in simple and choice reaction time tasks between obese and healthy-weight children. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34:2635–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Lindqvist P, Andersson K, Sundh V, Lissner L, Björkelund C, Bengtsson C. Concurrent and separate effects of body mass index and waist-to-hip ratio on 24-year mortality in the Population Study of Women in Gothenburg: evidence of age-dependency. Eur J Epidemiol. 2006;21:789–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Roche A, Heymsfield SB, Lohman TE. Body Composition. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 1996.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Welford AT. Choice reaction time: basic concepts. In: Welford AT, editor. Reaction Times. New York: Academic; 1980. p. 73–128.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Eckner JT, Kutcher JS, Richardson JK. Pilot evaluation of a novel clinical test of reaction time in National Collegiate Athletic Association Division I football players. J Athl Train. 2010;45:327–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Eckner JT, Whitacre RD, Kirsch N, Richardson JK. Evaluating a clinical measure of reaction time. Arch Physcal Med Rehab. 2006;87:10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Kowalski KC, Crocker PRE, Faulkner RA. Validation of the physical activity questionnaire for older children. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 1997;9:174–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Kowalski KC, Crocker PRE, Donen RMD. The physical activity questionnaire for older children (PAQ-C) and adolescents (PAQ-A) manual. Retrieved on 2007; from http://www.hkin.educ.ubc.ca/behavioural/PAQ%20manual.pdf.

  28. Crocker PRE, Bailey DA, Faukner RA, Kowalski KC, McGrath R. Measuring general levels of physical activity: Preliminary evidence for the physical activity questionnaire for older children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1997;29:1344–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Esmaeilzadeh S. Relationship between depressive symptoms with physical activity and physical fitness among children. Ment Health Prev. 2014;2:11–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Esmaeilzadeh S. The association between depressive symptoms and physical status including physical activity, aerobic and muscular fitness tests in children. Environ Health Prev Med. 2015;20:434–40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Kalantari HA, Esmaeilzadeh S. Association between academic achievement and physical status including physical activity, aerobic and muscular fitness tests in adolescent boys. Environ Health Prev Med. 2016;21:27–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Kamijo K, Pontifex MB, Khan NA, Raine LB, Scudder MR, Drollette ES, et al. The association of childhood obesity to neuroelectric indices of inhibition. Psychophysiology. 2012;49:1361–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Nooyens AC, Koppes LL, Visscher TL, Twisk JW, Kemper HC, Schuit AJ, et al. Adolescent skinfold thickness is a better predictor of high body fatness in adults than is body mass index: the Amsterdam Growth and Health Longitudinal Study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;85:1533–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Sarría A, García-Llop LA, Moreno LA, Fleta J, Morellón MP, Bueno M. Skinfold thickness measurements are better predictors of body fat percentage than body mass index in male Spanish children and adolescents. Eur J Clin Nutr. 1998;52:573–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Grantham J, Henneberg M. Adiposity is associated with improved neuromuscular reaction time. Med Hypotheses. 2014;83:593–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Gunstad J, Lhotsky A, Wendell CR, Ferrucci L, Zonderman AB. Longitudinal examination of obesity and cognitive function: results from the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Neuroepidemiology. 2010;34:222–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Nil.

Authors’ contributions

SE designed the study, helped for the measurements and data collection, performed the statistical analyses and wrote the manuscript and submitted it. AM helped for the measurements and data collection. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

No funding was received for performing the present study.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Human Ethics Committee of the Education Department and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Samad Esmaeilzadeh.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Moradi, A., Esmaeilzadeh, S. Simple reaction time and obesity in children: whether there is a relationship?. Environ Health Prev Med 22, 2 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12199-017-0612-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12199-017-0612-0

Keywords